Scalable representation of time in the hippocampus

Posted by
Spread the love
Scalable representation of time in the hippocampus
Scalable time representation of CA1 neurons in the temporal bisection task. (A) Schematic of the task (task 1). The rats ran on the treadmill for long or short intervals and then selected the left or right arm associated with water reward for long or short intervals, respectively. Sets of intervals were 10 s (long) and 5 s (short) in blocks 1 and 3 and 20 s (long) and 10 s (short) in block 2. (B, a to c) Firing patterns of three representative time cells in blocks 1 (left), 2 (center), and 3 (right). Top: Raster plots of long- and short-interval trials. Bottom: PETHs of long (solid)– and short (dotted line)–interval trials. (C) Firing patterns of time cells in blocks 1 (left), 2 (center), and 3 (right) (n = 454 units from seven rats). Each row represents PETHs of single neurons during long-interval trials in each block. The color scale represents the firing rate of each neuron. The neurons were ordered according to the peak time in block 1. (D) Population vector analyses of the data (C). Left: Autocorrelation of the population vector matrix of block 1. Center and right: Cross-correlations of blocks 1 and 2 and blocks 1 and 3, respectively. The color scale represents Pearson’s correlation coefficient. (E) Distributions of the scaling factors of time cells between blocks 1 and 2 (blue) and blocks 1 and 3 (green). Credit: Science Advances, doi:10.1126/sciadv.abd7013
Earn Bitcoin
Earn Bitcoin

Hippocampal time cells can encode specific moments of organized experiences in time to support hippocampal functions of episodic memory. But little is known about the reorganization of time cells during timely changes of episodes. Akihiro Shimbo and a research team at the Riken center for brain science and the department of psychology, Keio University in Japan investigated CA1 neuronal activity – that are critical for autobiographical memory, mental time travel and autonoetic consciousness in humans. During temporal bisection tasks they designed the sets of time intervals to be extended, discriminated or contracted, across the blocks of trials. The team scaled the theta phase precession and theta sequences of time cells to preserve the fine temporal relationships between pairs in theta cycles. Using rat models, they showed how the theta sequences reflected the decisions of rates on the basis of their time. The findings demonstrated how scalable features of time cells may support flexible temporal representations of memory formation.

Cognitive maps

Neuroscientists propose animals to have a cognitive map to serve as an internal representation of the relationships between entities in the world and to support flexible behavior. The hippocampus can provide cognitive maps for spatial navigation since hippocampal cell assemblies can organize map-like environmental representations. Hippocampal place cells use both rate and temporal (time) coding for space representation. Rate coding takes the form of receptive fields of locations. Spike timings of place cells can be strongly modulated by concurrent theta oscillations of 4 to 10 Hz, while exhibiting spike phase shifts as a function of distance relative to the center of the receptive field. With these two forms of rate and temporal coding, spatial representations of the hippocampus involved both positional and relational information. Relative positional relationships can provide more fundamental information for spatial recognition in the hippocampus than absolute positions. The hippocampus also provides a cognitive map for nonspatial entities including time-based information. However, not many studies are focused on investigating the systematic reorganization of representations of time in the hippocampus. In this work, Shimbo et al. studied the scalability of time in the hippocampus. The hippocampus represents information of elapsed time by the sequential activity of time cells. Previous reports had detailed how several brain regions including the prefrontal cortex and midbrain were involved in the perception and representation of elapsed time.

Scalable representation of time in the hippocampus
Scalability of theta phase precession of time cells. (A, a to f) Firing rates and theta phases of three representative time cells in blocks 1 (left column), 2 (center column), and 3 (right column) in task 1. Top: Raster plots of short and long-interval trials. Middle: PETHs of long (solid line)– and short (dotted line)–interval trials. Bottom: Theta phase plots as a function of elapsed time. Each dot represents an action potential from the long (light color)– and short (dark color)–interval trials. The black line in each panel represents circular-linear fits of the data (see Materials and Methods). (B) Relationships of the slopes of phase precession between blocks 1 and 2 (blue dots) and between blocks 1 and 3 (green dots) in each unit in task 1 (n = 351 units). Solid lines represent the fit of the linear regression of these data. (C) Slope of phase precession in each cell in block 2 divided by that in block 1 (blue). Slope of phase precession in each cell in block 3 divided by that in block 1 (green). Means ± SEM. ***P < 0.001, paired t test. Credit: Science Advances, doi:10.1126/sciadv.abd7013

The experiments

During the work, the researchers performed bisection tasks for rats to discriminate between long and short intervals to receive a reward. They noted the majority of time cells in the CA1 region to represent temporal information at scale across trial blocks. Shimbo et al. investigated the temporal structures of sequence activity of time cells associated with theta oscillations in the hippocampus to identify the mechanisms underlying the scalable representation of time. When analyzing the data, they noted the activities of time cells to reflect the rats’ decisions on the basis of their perception of time. The findings showed a common circuit mechanism that underlay map-like representations of spatial and non-spatial information in the hippocampus due to the similarities between place cells and time cells. The scientists used high-density silicon probes to record from the hippocampal CA1 neurons in the rats during the task behavior. They categorized the units as putative pyramidal cells or interneurons on the basis of their waveforms and firing rates.

Scalable representation of time in the hippocampus
Pairwise analysis of the temporal compression of spike sequences in single theta cycles. (A, a and b) Left: Firing rates and theta phases of two representative pairs of time cells (red and blue) in blocks 1 and 2 (task 1). The arrows indicate the peak firing rates. Right: Cross-correlograms (CCGs) of the pairs in blocks 1 (orange) and 2 (purple). The arrows indicate the peaks of the CCGs. Note that the CCG peak time shifts in (Ab) were larger than those in (Aa) (right), reflecting larger distances in PETH peak times of the pairs in (Ab) than those in (Aa) (left). a.u., arbitrary units. (B) Cross-correlations of PETHs of the pairs of time cells. (C) CCGs of the pairs of time cells in blocks 1 and 2 (n = 694 pairs). The orders of the pairs are the same in (B) and (C). (D) Pairwise analysis of the temporal compression of spike sequences in single theta cycles in blocks 1 and 2. Each dot represents a single neuronal pair. Horizontal axes show peak distances of PETHs, and vertical axes show peak shifts of the CCGs. The slopes of the regression line (red) were 29.2 and 15.0 ms/s in blocks 1 and 2, respectively. (E) Relationship between the time shifts of the CCGs from blocks 1 and 2. The slope of the regression line (red) was 0.98. Credit: Science Advances, doi:10.1126/sciadv.abd7013

Scalable representation of time

Shimbo et al. trained the rats on the temporal bisection task and set time intervals to discriminate between blocks of trials. During the experiments, they forced the rats to run on a treadmill for long or short durations of time. After the forced run, they selected the left or right arm in a Y maze. The researchers provided water drops as a reward at the completion of each task at the correct arm. They first assessed the time-dependent activities of pyramidal cells and categorized 22.2 percent of pyramidal cells as time cells. They also determined if the time representation of CA1 pyramidal cells were scalable to a shrinking direction—to indicate the time representation of CA1 pyramidal cells to be scalable in both expanding and shrinking directions.

Task dependency on the scalable representation of time

To test how the scalable features of time also depended on task demands, Shimbo et al. trained another set of rats on light discrimination tasks in which the animals were not required to discriminate between time intervals. During the tasks, they forced the rats to run on a treadmill at short or long intervals after which they required the animals to choose the left or right arm of the Y maze in a light-guided manner. They then investigated the CA1 neuronal activity in rats performing the light discrimination tasks. The researchers classified 7.5 percent of the pyramidal cells as time cells, which were significantly lower than those from the temporal bisection tasks. Based on the outcome, the formation of time cells depended on the demands of the task, while also having scalable representation of elapsed time information.

Scalable representation of time in the hippocampus
Scalability of theta sequences of time cells. (A) Decoded elapsed-time information at each time point in a single example trial in task 1. Top: Simultaneously recorded LFPs (wide band and 4 to 10 Hz) and units from 10 pyramidal neurons ordered by the peak times of their firing rates from bottom to top. Bottom: Probability density of temporal information was computed from simultaneously recorded pyramidal cell activity in the trial. Bayesian decoding was performed at every 1-ms time step with a 20-ms time window. (B) Probability densities of the decoded time at each theta phase in blocks 1, 2, and 3 in the example single session in task 1. Horizontal axes represent theta phases in which spiking data were sampled, and vertical axes represent time decoded from spiking activity relative to the sampled time. The result indicates that decoded relative time was extended vertically in block 2. (C) Scaling factors of theta sequences between blocks 1 and 2 and blocks 1 and 3 (n = 26 sessions; sessions that contained more than eight time cells were selected). Means ± SEM. **P < 0.01, paired t test. Credit: Science Advances, doi:10.1126/sciadv.abd7013

Scalability of theta phase precession and theta sequence of time cells

Hippocampal cell assemblies generated sequential structures during space navigation in single theta cycles known as theta sequences. These cycles compressively represented trajectory sequences of the past, current and future positions. Shimbo et al. checked for the presence of theta phase precession as a function of elapsed time during the temporal bisection task. The local field potential was dominated by a robust theta frequency (4 to 10 Hz) oscillation during the interval period. The team studied the phase-time relationships of single neurons and found that 77.3 percent of the time cells presented phase precession. Shimbo et al. investigated the temporal structure of spike sequences of time cell assemblies in single theta cycles to understand if time cell assemblies formed theta sequences during the task.

The results presented temporal relationships of cell pairs in single theta cycles. The team then assessed if the theta sequences of the time cells were merely consequences of phase precession of individual neurons, or if they had higher correlative structures than those predicted from phase precession. The results showed that the theta sequences of the time cells were not merely the consequence of phase precession of individual neurons. Together, these outcomes showed how the theta sequence of time cells were scalable across the trial blocks and how fine temporal relationships between cell pairs were conserved within single theta cycles. The scientists also studied how the assembly of time cells reflected recognition of elapsed time in the rats. The work showed how these results indicated the activity of time cell assemblies and actually reflected animals’ decisions during the test trials.

Scalable representation of time in the hippocampus
Scalability of rate and temporal coding of time cells. Representations of time information were scalable in rate and temporal coding in the hippocampus. Regarding fine temporal structures of spiking activity in single theta cycles, the temporal relationships of units are preserved between short and long episodes. Regarding neuronal representations of elapsed-time information in single theta cycles, the temporal representations of spike sequences are scaled between short and long episodes. Credit: Science Advances, doi:10.1126/sciadv.abd7013

Outlook

In this way, Akihiro Shimbo and colleagues investigated the scalability of time cells in the hippocampus relative to rate and temporal coding. The results indicated scalability for temporal representation in the hippocampus and hippocampal pyramidal cells relative to the two factors of rate and time. The work suggests an underlying common circuit mechanism with map-like representations of spatial and nonspatial information in the hippocampus. Such scalable, map-like architectures of space-time information will allow for the formation of flexible memory to adapt to complex environments.

Thamarasee Jeewandara , Medical Xpress